Animal Behavior Laboratory Manual
MOCKINGBIRD TERRITORIALITY AND SONG

Lab Manual Table of Contents

Northern Mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos) are year-round residents
in North Carolina and defend territories throughout the year. In
the winter, a female sometimes defends a separate territory adjacent to
that of her mate from the previous summer, or a pair sometimes defends a
territory together throughout the year. Males and females can only
be distinguished by behavioral differences. In particular, only
males sing.
A mockingbird spends nearly all of its time within its territory.
In behavioral studies, a territory is a defended area used
exclusively (or at least mostly) by the resident and its mate. A
great variety of different species, vertebrates and invertebrates, defend
territories. In some species a territory provides only some of an
individual's requirements, such as a nest site or food, but not both.
In these cases, territorial individuals must of course leave their
territories to obtain their other necessities.
In contrast, a mockingbird's territory, like those of many other small
songbirds, includes all of its requirements: places to feed, rest,
sleep, and seek shelter from predators. During the breeding season,
nesting also occurs within the territory. Consequently, each
mockingbird spends nearly all of its time within its own territory.
Mockingbirds feed mostly on insects during spring and summer and fruit
during fall and winter. The red berries of the holly trees around
Coker and Wilson Halls are favorite sources of food during winter.
A mockingbird defends its territory primarily against other mockingbirds,
but sometimes it also attacks other birds that feed on fruit inside its
territory. Why is fruit guarded so jealously? If food
becomes too scarce during winter, a mockingbird must occasionally leave
its own territory and fend for itself in other birds' territories, where
it is subject to attack whenever discovered.
Territorial animals defend their territories by means of outright attacks
and fights when necessary. Much more frequent, however, are
displays which serve to advertise that an individual has claimed and is
prepared to defend an area.
These advertisement displays take diverse forms in animals:
many territorial mammals use scents (secretions from special glands or
simply urine or feces) to mark boundaries or conspicuous points in their
territories. Many territorial birds use long-range sounds to
advertise territories. Most birds' songs, including the
mockingbird's songs, fall into this category. A male mockingbird
uses conspicuous perches as song posts from which to proclaim its presence
on a territory.
Males sing primarily from January (warm days only) through July and then
again in October. From November through February, they use another
distinctive sound, a sudden harsh chat or chat-chat-chat, to
proclaim their territories especially early in the morning. A
tape-recording of a mockingbird's songs or a burst of chats, when
played from a speaker inside a mockingbird's territory, often elicits a
prompt aggressive response from the resident. What might explain
this behavior?
In this exercise, you will
- map a mockingbird's territory
- determine the proportion of time it spends in different activities
(its time budget)
- observe behavior used to advertise and to defend its territory
- conduct an experiment to determine some characteristics of stimuli
that evoke territorial aggression
Procedure -- Advance Preparation
Field work almost always requires preparation in advance. A week
before we actually start to study mockingbirds, you will have to get
ready:
- load batteries into the cassette tape players and the
speaker/amplifiers. (Also remove the batteries the week after they
are used.)
- learn how to connect the tape-recorder to the speaker/amplifier and
how to adjust the volume.
- design a data sheet to collect information on time budgets and
another one to collect information on the responses to playback.
All classes might use the same standardized sheet -- but everyone should
think about how to design it and perhaps should prepare their own sheets.
- check the binoculars. Learn how to focus both eyepieces and to
adjust the distance between eyepieces so you see a single, clearly focused
circle. You should check them for alignment by holding the binocs a
foot or so in front of your eyes and focusing on some distant horizontal
line (the eves of a building, for instance). If the binocs are in
alignment, the horizontal line will appear at the same level in both
eyepieces.
- learn how to identify mockingbirds! They are gray birds with
conspicuous white spots in the wings and white edges of the tail.
Many (but not all) mockingbirds on campus have colored plastic bands on
their legs.
Procedure -- Week I
Work in groups of 4-5. Use a paper and pencil for recording
observations, a watch to time behavior, and binoculars. Each group
will record the behavior of one mockingbird (or a pair) for one hour.
Two students will serve as observers with responsibility for keeping the
bird in sight (or knowing its location, if it is in a bush out of sight)
at all times. A third person will record the locations of the bird
on a map. The fourth person will keep a written account of the
bird's behavior, and the fifth person will serve as timer.
Everybody should take a turn looking at your subject with the binoculars
-- does it have colored plastic bands on its legs?
In recording the behavior of an animal, we need to take into account two
kinds of activites. Behavioral events are actions that take
place essentially instantaneously (or are too brief to allow us to time
their duration), such as eating an item of food or attacking an opponent.
Behavioral states are activities that last long enough for
us to time their duration, such as resting on a perch, singing or
preening continuously, or displaying repeatedly to a neighbor at a
boundary. When you make a record of your mockingbird's activities,
you will want to count all events but time the duration of
all states.
To estimate the time an individual spends in different states, you should
use scan sampling, the procedure used in the exercise on fish
schooling. Instead of recording the time at the start and stop of
each behavioral state, record the bird's behavioral state every 10 seconds
(the person serving as timer can help here). In one hour you will
then have 6 observations/minute X 60 minutes = 360 scan observations at
10-second intervals. How can you use this information to estimate
the proportion of time spent in each behavioral state?
You also will want to pay attention to a bird's locations and
conspicuousness. Perching on a high bare branch (conspicuous both
to other mockingbirds and possible predators) is not likely to be
equivalent, from the point of view of a mockingbird, to perching out of
sight in the middle of a dense bush. Think like your mockingbird
when you decide what to record about its behavior!
Take notes that will allow you to determine the following:
- Proportion of time spent by your subject in different behavioral
states: perching in conspicuous places, perching out of sight (but
in a location that you know for sure), preening, singing, foraging for
food on the ground, in trees, and so forth)
- Rates of behavioral events. Rates are frequencies/minute of
observation (or 10 minutes of observation, if more convenient):
food items eaten, flights, chases or swoops at mockingbirds, chases of
other species, sun-bathing, wing-flashing while hunting for food on the
ground, chat calls, and so forth
- A map of the bird's conspicuous perches, other perches, and feeding
sites
Especially important are the locations at which your subject attacks or
challenges opponents (if any). Also include the locations of other
mockingbirds that you notice while watching your subject. Then
determine the area defended by your subject, the area used exclusively by
your subject, and the total area used by your subject. Do these
three areas coincide?
After recording your observations for one hour, prepare the above three
summaries. You should have made a start in understanding your
subject's territorial behavior. Give your summaries to your
Teaching Assistant.
Procedure -- Week II
This week you will conduct an experiment with playbacks of
tape-recordings. During the week or two before this exercise, you
will have a chance to check all the equipment we use.
Be sure that all of the equipment works and that you understand
how it works.
During this exercise, you will again observe your bird's behavior for one
hour and make a map of its territory, in order to obtain some additional
information about your subject's behavior and the limits of the territory.
Afterwards, you will use playbacks of tape-recordings to create the
impression of different kinds of intruders in the territory.
Each group will present two different tapes, selected from the four listed
below. Different groups will present different combinations, so you
can compare the mockingbirds' responses to all four tapes:
- a mockingbird's songs near the center of the territory
- a mockingbird's songs near (but inside) the boundary
- a mockingbird's chat calls near the center of the territory
- another common species' songs near the center of the territory (we
use the Carolina Wren, a species nearly as numerous as the Northern
Mockingbird on campus)
Each tape has 10 minutes of recording. You should make observations
for 10 minutes before and after the playback as well (see below).
Then wait at least 10 minutes, during which you should move the speaker at
least 15 m (50 feet), before the next playback. If appropriate,
after selecting positions for the speaker, flip a coin to determine which
tape and which position to use first.
The loudness of the playback is a important variable in these experiments.
Too quiet and the playback might not be heard. Too loud and
the mockingbird might conclude that its territory has just been invaded by
the mockingbird equivalent of Godzilla! Encourage students to use
their own ears during the playbacks to adjust the volume. It is no
use adjusting volume in the lab (sound reverberates in the room and makes
any estimates of loudness useless outdoors).
As before, use scan sampling to estimate durations of behavioral
states and counts to determine the frequencies of behavioral
events. For these experiments, you should also record the
latencies of conspicuous responses (the time from the start of
playback until the subject's first reaction).
Use these procedures to obtain at least the following four measures of
your subject's behavior, during the three intervals (10 minutes before
playback starts, 10 minutes during playback, and 10 minutes after playback
ends), for each playback:
- time spent within 10 m (just over 10 yards) of the speaker
- latency to approach within 10 m after the start of playback
- closest approach during playback
- number of songs.
Did some states or events increase during or after playback? Or
decrease? By subtracting the measures of your subject's behavior
before playback from the same measures during and after playback, you can
determine how much its behavior changed.
Use the results from your entire section (or all sections) to compare
behavior before, during, and after the playback. Use simple
statistics (see the supplement to this manual).
Also compare responses to the three different kinds of playback.
How would you use simple statistics in this case?
Questions
Do mockingbirds respond to conspecific (same species) calls as much as to
songs?
Do they respond to other species' songs as much as to conspecific songs?
How do responses to playbacks resemble responses to natural intruders?
How do they differ?
Why do certain stimuli evoke territorial aggression?
Why do we record data before and after the playback as well as during it?
Why do we choose positions for the speaker and then flip a coin to
determine which to use first?
Why do we move the speaker between playbacks?

References
Breitwisch, R., M. Diaz, N. Gottlieb, R. Lee, and J. Zaias. 1986.
Defense of fall territories by mated and unmated northern
mockingbirds in southern Florida. Journal of Field Ornithology 57:
16-21.
Breitwisch, R., M. Diaz, and R. Lee. 1987. Foraging
efficiencies and techniques of juvenile and adult northern mockingbirds
(Mimus polyglottos). Behaviour 101: 225-235.
Derrickson, K. C. 1987. Behavioral correlates of song types
of the northern mockingbird (Mimus polyglottos). Ethology
74: 21-32.
Derrickson, K.C. 1987. Yearly and situational changes in the
estimate of repertoire size in Northen Mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos).
Ethology 74: 21-32.
Derrickson, K.C. and Breitwisch, R. 1992. Northern
Mockingbird (Mimus polygottos). In A. Poole, P. Stettenheim, and F.
Gill, Eds. Birds of North America. Philadelphia, Academy of
Natural Sciences, and Washington, D.C., American Ornithologist's Union.
Pp. 2-26.
Godard, R. 1991. Long-term memory of individual neighbours
in a migratory songbird. Nature 350: 228-229.
Godard, R. 1993. Tit for tat among neighboring hooded
warblers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 33: 45-50.
Hailman, J. P. 1960. Hostile dancing and fall territory of a
color-banded mockingbird. Condor 62: 464-468.
Howard, R. D. 1974. The influence of sexual selection and
interspecific competitioon on mockingbird song. Evolution 28:
428-438.
Laskey, A. R. 1935. Mockingbird life history studies.
Auk 57: 370-381.
Laskey, A. R. 1962. Breeding biology of mockingbirds.
Auk 79: 596-606.
Logan, C. 1983. Reproductively dependent song cyclicity in
mated male mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos). Auk 100:
404-413.
Logan, C. 1985. Mockingbird use of chatbursts with neighbors
versus strangers. Journal of Field Ornithology 56: 69-71.
Logan, C.A. and Hyatt, L.E. 1991. Mate attraction by
autumnal song in the northern mockingbird (Mimus polyglottos). Auk
108: 429-432.
Logan, C.A., Hyatt, L.E., and Gregorcyk, L. 1990. Song
playback initiates nest building during clutch overlap in mockingbirds
(Mimus polyglottos). Animal Behavior 39: 943-953.
Selander, R., and D. Hunter. 1960. On the functions of
wingflashing in mockingbirds. Wilson Bulletin 72: 340-345.

|