Animal Behavior Laboratory Manual
DISPLAYS AND HABITUATION OF PARADISE FISH

Lab Manual Table of Contents

Introduction
Early ethologists, especially Oskar Heinroth and Konrad Lorenz in Germany
and Julian Huxley in England, recognized that many animals have a number
of distinct action patterns used in social interactions.
These action patterns, sometimes called displays, often seem bizarre and
mechanical to our eyes.
Because these displays are often repeated in almost the same way,
performance after performance, and because different individuals of the
same species often perform the displays almost the same way, they have
often been called fixed action patterns. In some cases they
are fixed in another way also: they are not learned from other
members of the species; instead each individual develops these displays
without having observed them previously.
Careful study has revealed that these action patterns do vary, sometimes
considerably. Nevertheless, they are often easily recognizable
because the animals rarely perform any action intermediate between
different displays. Because they are not "fixed", perhaps the best
term is simply action pattern.
In this exercise, we investigate the displays of paradise fish
(Macropodus opercularis). Once you learn to recognize their
displays, try to observe how variable and how discrete they are.
In addition, we examine a general feature of behavior -- habituation.
Habituation is a decrease in the intensity of response to a
constant or repeated stimulus -- a decrease not explained by muscular
fatigue (muscles no longer respond after repeated stimulation) or
sensory adaptation (sensory neurons no longer respond after
repeated stimulation). Habituation is a result of changes in the
central nervous system, not in sensory or muscle cells.
To study habituation, we must measure the intensity of responses to a
stimulus, for example a mirror image or a picture. Some measures of
response intensity are
- the rate of response (number of actions per minute)
- the latency
of response (time between the presentation of a stimulus and the first
response)
- the duration of response (for instance, seconds of response
per minute of observation).
We use some of these measures in our investigation of habituation.
Procedures
Your fish have been visually isolated for about two weeks in individual
tanks. Organize yourselves in groups of two. Each pair of
observers will have one fish.
Observe the typical resting behavior of the fish. Note the
general body postures, the color patterns, the normal rates of gill
movement, the normal rate of rising to the surface for air (these fish
belong to an Asian family that can breathe air), and the rate of pectoral
fin fanning. Time these actions with a stopwatch.
Study the reaction to mirrors. Place a mirror at one end of
the tank so that it covers the entire end. Observe the changes in
behavior from the resting state. Observe the fish for 15 minutes.
Measure the intensity of responses each minute (see above).
Is there a tendency to habituate to the mirror? Are the displays
performed the same way each time? How much do they vary from
performance to performance? Can you recognize more and less intense
versions of each display? How could you rule out muscular fatigue
or sensory adaptation as explanations for the decrease in responses?
Observe the behavior of two fish together. Combine with
another group to make a group of four. Each team of two should keep
track of the behavior of one of the two fish. Put two fish together
(remember which ones they are so you can compare their behavior to that in
the mirror test). Typically they approach and circle slowly around
each other. After this approach and circle, a complete aggressive
encounter usually includes these behavior patterns: (a) frontal
display; (b) lateral displays; (c) lateral attack; (d) head ram; and (f)
jaw lock. The encounter sometimes does not proceed all the way to
attack and fighting.
Are the displays performed in a fixed sequence? Do less intense
displays tend to precede more intense ones? What happens if one
fish becomes subordinate and the other dominant?
Descriptions of Displays
- Frontal display
- gill covers (opercula) erected, presenting a larger frontal profile to
the opponent.
- Lateral display
- four major components include antiparallel head-to-tail orientation,
expansion of the dorsal and caudal fins, S-shaping of the body, and
quivering or vibration along with rapid beating of the pectoral fins.
- Lateral attack
- a glancing attack directed at the flank or side, usually at the base
of the opponent's tail; normally seen at the end of the vibration that
terminates lateral display.
- Frontal attack
- biting directed at the head, mouth or operculum of the opponent.
- Head ram
- self-explanatory.
- Jaw lock
- one male grabs his opponent's lower jaw and a wrestling contest
ensues; often preceded by head ramming.
- Subordinate behavior
- fins folded, body axis at 45 degrees to horizontal (head upward),
color change to a lighter shade, little motion except when chased.
Questions
Why do these fish display to each other rather than attack without
displaying?
Why do you think they respond differently to mirrors and live opponents?
How variable are these displays? How does variation in the
performances of one fish compare with variation between different
individuals? Are sequences of displays more or less variable than
the displays themselves?
How many distinct action patterns have you recognized in interactions of
two male paradise fish? Why are there not more, or fewer?
Why is it adaptive for an animal to habituate to a constant or repeated
stimulus?

References
Bronstein, P.M. 1994. On the predictability,
sensitization
and habituation of aggression in male Bettas (Betta splendens). J.
Comp. Psych. 108(1): 45-57.
Francis, R. C. 1983. Experiential effects on agonistic
behavior in the paradise fish, Macropodus opercularis.
Behaviour 85: 292-313.
Francis, R. C. 1984. The effects of bidirectional selection
for social dominance on agonistic behavior and sex ratios in the paradise
fish (Macropodus opercularis). Behaviour 90: 25-45.
Francis, R. C. 1987. The interaction of genotype and
experience in the dominance success of paradise fish (Macropodus
opercularis). Biology of Behaviour 12: 1-11.
Miklosi, A., J. Haller, and V. Csanyi. Learning about the opponent
during aggressive encounters in paradise fish (Macropodus opercularis L.):
When it takes place? Behavioural Processes 40: 97-105.
Rasa, A. O. 1969. Territoriality and the establishment of
dominance by means of visual cues in Pomacentrus jenkensi .
Zeitschrift fŸr Tierpsychologie 26: 825-845. (shelved with
the journal Ethology in the Zoology Library)
Thompson, T. I. 1963. Visual reinforcement in Siamese
fighting fish. Science 141: 55-57.

|